They
are among us. They look just like us, but
they are not just like us. They
are hosts to the Invaders, beings who lodge themselves inside human brains,
controlling behaviour for their own primordial ends. The elderly lady beside
you on the bus who smells faintly of cat-pee and mothballs – she could be one
of Them. The student behind you, snuffling loudly and popping another Panadol
from the packet – is he one? The orange girl nattering into her mobile with one
hand, plucking fine white hairs out of her scarf with the other – is it her? Indeed,
given the statistics, the chances are that at least one member of the CogSci
class is host to behaviour-modifying t.gondii
oocysts. Even you, gentle reader, could be one of Them.
There
are a number of parasite species which control the behaviour of their hosts
(here are a few examples). The brain
parasite toxoplasma gondii is one of
the most famous, renowned for its capacity to make rodents fear cats less. T.gondii has evolved this capacity because
it can only complete its life-cycle in the body of a cat; in other mammals it
re-encysts, forming an oocyst in the host’s brain. In humans, the effect of
toxoplasma oocysts include ‘increased intelligence, superego strength and
affectothymia’ in women, and ‘decreased intelligence, superego strength and
novelty seeking in men’ (Lafferty, 2006: 2749). Infection, in some parts of the
world, is quite rampant, and some estimates suggest that up to half the global
population is infected. For example, 25% of the Irish population are infected,
and 38% of all UK meat products contain toxoplasma cysts. Infection occurs
through consumption of contaminated meat products or contact with kitty poo,
among other means. Initial symptoms are brief and flu-like, and then everything
seems normal again. And as if the individual consequences weren’t creepy enough,
this paper by Kevin Lafferty suggests that human cultures, and thus, we may
infer, the course of history, have been altered by toxoplasma.
‘Culture’
is probably one of the most vaguely defined terms around; in fact, you could
even say it rivals ‘cognition’. Despite this vagueness, one thing most people
would probably be comfortable enough asserting is that human culture is an
almost exclusively human product - it is something that arises out of our
nature, our collective history, our relationship to the world around us and our
relationships with others. However, according to Lafferty, toxoplasma gondii may actually have affected the way human cultures
have developed through the aggregation at the ‘national level’ of toxoplasma-induced
personality changes which occur at the individual level. Lafferty found that,
in western countries, increased toxoplasma prevalence correlated with cultural ‘neuroticism,
novelty-avoidance and masculine domination’. I am not deeply convinced of the
worth of these rather crude ‘cultural dimensions’, which seem redolent of 1930’s
‘Culture and Personality’ school anthropology, and it is also worth emphasising
that the correlations did not hold for non-western societies, leading Lafferty
to conclude that if toxoplasma does have an effect at the cultural level, it is
likely only a very limited one (Japan, for example, has very low toxoplasma
prevalence but very high ‘novelty avoidance’). However, cognitively speaking, three
central points remain of interest. One is the implication that personalities
are the product, not just of human beings, but of other organisms acting
through human beings. Another is the idea that personalities can aggregate into
group-level traits, which are presumably self-reinforcing. Thus, we are robbed
of our Lutheran atomism from above and below. The last is what toxoplasma
infection says about our notions of being unified agents in possession of ‘free
will’.
Philosopher
Daniel Dennett here eloquently expounds his view that the brain is, in fact, a chaos of competing agencies; each neuron constitutes an agent in its own right,
and each screams to be heard above the clamour of its brothers. This view
dissolves the individual into a legion and could be accused of wiping away any
hope of free will, but at least we can still say that the sub-agents are in
some sense native components of ‘ourselves’. It is initially alarming but
ultimately bearable. Indeed, the idea that our behaviour is influenced by
hidden agencies beneath the level of our experience has become fairly widely
accepted since Freud, and as for personal unity, the great 18th
century philosopher Hume famously claimed that the self was nothing but a
bundle of perceptions. However the thought that a significant percentage of us
are actually composite beings whose behaviour is influenced by invasive organisms
lodged within our brains, microscopic cat-poo puppet masters against whom even
the most reflective of tinfoil hats can offer no protection, is far more
chilling.
oh parasitology.
ReplyDeletereading this provoked a thought: many people are familiar with the radical (albeit biologically and evolutionarily incorrect) assertion that a fetus is a parasite while it is inside its mother. nevertheless, pregnancy radically alters the behavior of the mother through biological and hormonal means. other behavioral alterations (such as dietary changes), are directly caused by the presence of the fetus but are made of the mother's own volition--these choices can be made for the fetus's benefit or detriment. while obviously not mind "control", certainly mind alteration occurs at both conscious and sub-conscious levels.
are unborn children a biologically-reconcilable version of the Cartesian homonculus? no. but in certain circumstances from a certain point of view, kinda :)